Uncategorized

TWO.ZERO.ONE.ZERO - Lyrische Interpretation (German Edition)

This happy period of his life, during which he produced the most charming and spontaneous of his love-lyrics, came to an end with the death of Duke Frederick in Henceforward Walther was a wanderer from court to court, singing for his lodging and his bread, and ever hoping that some patron would arise to save him from this "juggler's life" gougel-fuore and the shame of ever playing the guest. He had few if any possessions and depended on others for his food and lodging. His criticism of men and manners was scathing; and even when this did not touch his princely patrons, their underlings often took measures to rid themselves of so uncomfortable a censor.

Thus he was forced to leave the court of the generous duke Bernhard of Carinthia — ; after an experience of the tumultuous household of the landgrave of Thuringia , he warns those who have weak ears to give it a wide berth. Generosity could be mentioned by Walther von der Vogelweide. Ich bin dem Bogenaere Katzenelnbogener holt — gar ane gabe und ane solt: Walther was, in fact, a man of strong views; and it is this which gives him his main significance in history, as compared to his place in literature.

From the moment when the death of the emperor Henry VI opened the fateful struggle between empire and papacy , Walther threw himself ardently into the fray on the side of German independence and unity. Although his religious poems sufficiently prove the sincerity of his Catholicism , he remained to the end of his days opposed to the extreme claims of the popes, whom he attacks with a bitterness which can be justified only by the strength of his patriotic feelings. His political poems begin with an appeal to Germany, written in at Vienna, against the disruptive ambitions of the princes: He was present in at Philip 's coronation at Mainz , and supported him till his victory was assured.

After Philip's murder in , he "said and sang" in support of Otto of Brunswick against the papal candidate Frederick of Hohenstaufen ; and only when Otto's usefulness to Germany had been shattered by the Battle of Bouvines did he turn to the rising star of Frederick, now the sole representative of German majesty against pope and princes. From the new emperor, Walther's genius and zeal for the empire finally received recognition: That Frederick gave him a further sign of favour by making him the tutor of his son Henry VII , King of the Romans , is more than doubtful.

The fact, in itself highly improbable, rests upon the evidence of only a single poem, the meaning of which can also be interpreted otherwise. Walther's restless spirit did not suffer him to remain long on his new property. In he was once more at Vienna, and again in after the return of Duke Leopold VI from the crusade.

He was active in urging the German princes to take part in the crusade of , and may have accompanied the crusading army at least as far as his native Tirol. In a poem he pictures in words the changes that had taken place in the scenes of his childhood, changes which made his life there seem to have been only a dream.

iTunes is the world's easiest way to organize and add to your digital media collection.

Walther's work is exceptionally well preserved compared to that of his contemporaries, with over 30 complete manuscripts and fragments containing widely varying numbers of strophes under his name. The most extensive collections of his songs are in four of the main Minnesang manuscripts: In addition to these, there are many manuscripts with smaller amounts of material, sometimes as little as a single strophe. With the exception of MS M the Carmina Burana , which may even have been compiled in Walther's lifetime, all the sources date from at least two generations after his death, and most are from the 14th or 15th centuries.

Certain or potential melodies to Walther's songs come from three sources: The latter are the only potential melodies to Walther's love songs, the remainder being for religious and political songs. The ascription of other melodies to Walther in the Meistersang manuscripts the Goldene Weise , the Kreuzton , and the Langer Ton is regarded as erroneous. How she carols over the heath in her high clear voice! What marvels she performs! How deftly she sings in organon! How she varies her singing from one compass to another in that mode, I mean, which has come down to us from Cythaeron , on whose slopes and in whose caves the Goddess of Love holds sway!

She is Mistress of the Chamber there at court. Grove Music Online evaluates Walther's work as follows:. He is regarded as one of the most outstanding and innovative authors of his generation His poetic oeuvre is the most varied of his time, In his work he freed Minnesang from the traditional patterns of motifs and restricting social function and transformed it into genuinely experienced and yet universally valid love-poetry.

Walther's main contribution to the German love lyric was to increase the range of roles that could be adopted by the singer and his beloved, and to lend the depiction of the experience of love new immediacy and vibrancy. In Walther's political and didactic poetry we again observe a consummately versatile poetic voice, one which finds new ways to give artistic expression to experience despite the constraints of the taste of audiences and patrons and by the authority of literary conventions.

Walther is one of the traditional competitors in the tale of the song contest at the Wartburg. Walter is mentioned in Samuel Beckett 's short story " The Calmative ": In , a statue of Walther was unveiled in a square in Bolzano see above , which was subsequently renamed the Walther von der Vogelweide-Platz. Under fascist rule, the statue was moved to a less prominent site, but it was restored to its original location in Fountain on main square in Sankt Veit an der Glan , Austria. There have been more scholarly editions of Walther's works than of any other medieval German poet's, a reflection of both his importance to literary history and the complex manuscript tradition.

Consistent reference to Walther's songs is made by means of "Lachmann numbers", which are formed of an "L" for "Lachmann" followed by the page and line number in Lachmann's edition of All serious editions and translations of Walther's songs either give the Lachmann numbers alongside the text or provide a concordance of Lachmann numbers for the poems in the edition or translation.

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. This section has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. Learn how and when to remove these template messages. This shows that readers' perceptions of the poems' general affective meaning generally correspond well with the author-given affective categorization. Valence and arousal ratings further support the character of these discrete affective categories.

For example, the author-defined spiteful poems show the strongest negative valence and highest arousal ratings for statistical comparisons see Aryani et al. Means M and standard deviations SD of the rating variables for each author-given affective category N being the number of poems rated. Especially valence is highly correlated with friendliness in a positive way, and with spitefulness in a negative way.

An opposite pattern is found for the correlation between arousal and spitefulness positive compared to friendliness negative. Whereas liking and poeticity correlate moderately with each other as well as with valence and friendliness, onomatopoeia, and sadness are the two ratings correlating least with the other ones. Bivariate correlations between valence ratings and four predictors: Higher valence ratings go along with increasing lexical valence values but decreasing lexical arousal values see Figures 1A,B.

This pattern is in line with the general negative correlation between the two affective dimensions in, for example, words from German affective word databases BAWL: Regarding the sublexical arousal level of all these salient segments, the inclusion of the absolute arousal values allows a more detailed characterization of the underlying mechanisms: This is confirmed by the absolute values of sublexical arousal of all salient segments showing a positive partial correlation. For high-arousing sublexical segments however, the two variables would predict opposite patterns which cancel out each other.

The absolute values of valence ratings, representing the intensity of valence ratings irrespective of their direction, can best be predicted by lexical arousal and the sublexical arousal values of all salient segments. This clearly reflects the U-shaped distribution of lexical valence and arousal values in affective word databases BAWL: Bradley and Lang, , where both the arousal levels of positive and negative words are higher than for neutral words, even if the arousal for positive words does not reach the same height as for negative words in the German language Schmidtke et al.

Bivariate correlations between the absolute values of valence ratings and two predictors: For the arousal ratings, no sublexical affective values appear in the regression model. The variance in these ratings is mainly accounted for by the word with the highest arousal level in the poem, but also by the overall lexical arousal level: But also a changing level of lexical valence throughout the poem has an influence on the perceived arousal: Bivariate correlations between arousal ratings and three predictors: Furthermore, a higher intensity of the sublexical valence values of all nuclei in the text—being represented by the absolute value—seems to lead to higher friendliness ratings.

Bivariate correlations between friendliness ratings and four predictors: Contrary to friendliness, in the spitefulness model, lower lexical valence and higher lexical arousal lead to higher spitefulness ratings Figures 5A,D , as could be expected for high arousing negative poems such as spiteful ones. This, however, might be a specific quality of this particular poem corpus, not being transferable into general.

Bivariate correlations between spitefulness ratings and four predictors: A first glance at the regression model for sadness shows that, unlike in every other of the analyzed models, neither lexical valence nor arousal per se is included. Another important inter-lexical aspect in the case of sadness is the correlation of word affectivity with the word order.

This is reflected by the absolute value of the correlation of lexical arousal with the words' positions entering the regression model as well, which neutralizes the potential influence of higher lexical arousal values. At the sublexical level, the absolute value of the arousal level of all codas in the text seems to be the strongest predictor. Thus, any coda's arousal value being significantly different from the distribution's mean—no matter whether it is especially low or high-arousing—leads to higher sadness ratings.

The same holds for the valence values of all types of salient segments in the poems. Furthermore, the occurrence of many salient nuclei in a text goes along with lower sadness rating. Bivariate correlations between sadness ratings and four predictors: Both types of arousal show a negative partial correlation with the dependent variable: Bivariate correlations between liking ratings and two predictors: For the dependent variable poeticity , lexical arousal appears as a highly significant predictor variable if its absolute values are considered: The more deviant the lexical arousal values are from zero, no matter whether into a higher arousing or more calming direction, the less poetic the poem is rated Figures 8A,B.

Thus, poems that contain predominantly words of a rather unremarkable arousal—not significantly high- or low-arousing—are perceived as more poetic than poems with salient lexical arousal features. Moreover, the poeticity ratings are also strongly influenced by sublexical affective values. This results from the finding that the continuous arousal values of the nuclei are negatively correlated with the poeticity ratings, while the absolute arousal values correlate in a positive manner.

Thus, for the negative range—namely the low-arousing part—the inferred statement is the same, whereas in the positive—high-arousing—range the correlation patterns oppose and hence zero out each other. Consequently, more arousing nuclei values do not necessarily lead to diminished poeticity ratings. Bivariate correlations between poeticity and four predictors: The onomatopoetic perception is significantly influenced by variables from all three text levels.

TWO.ZERO.ONE.ZERO - Lyrische Interpretation

At the lexical level, a higher occurrence of negatively valenced words in a poem leads to increased onomatopoeia ratings. In contrast, with a higher maximum value of lexical valence in a poem, the ratings for onomatopoeia become slightly higher as well. However, this partial correlation is not a very strong one. Regarding the spread of lexical valence and arousal in each poem—depicted by their standard deviations—higher deviations involve lower onomatopoeia ratings Figures 9A,B.

At the sublexical level, the nuclei seem to play an important role: The overall picture receives further complexity by the fact that a more positive valence specifically of salient codas augments the onomatopoeia ratings. Bivariate correlations between onomatopoeia ratings and four predictors: In summary, it can be stated that in all of the regression models at least two out of the three examined levels of affective text analysis contribute significantly but differently to the variance in the respective dependent rating variable. In eight out of nine cases, at least one of the lexical variables valence or arousal is contained in the regression model, in six cases it enters the model first.

Especially the inclusion of lexical arousal in seven models increases the amount of explained variance to a noticeable extent. Lexical valence supports four models significantly. The newly defined inter-lexical variables, whose task it is to represent dynamic shifts and spreads of affective lexical content, find their way into the regression equations in five out of nine models. From the huge number of sublexical predictor variables, prominently the arousal level of salient segments consistently explains variance in eight out of nine models.

In addition, the pure number of salient segments in a poem, disregarding their affective values, plays a role in four of the nine regression models. This study investigates to which extent affective connotations at the rather basic textual dimensions of phonological units and single words or the relative positions of the latter influence the overall affective perception of poetry. To estimate their affective perception by the reader, we collected ratings of the poems on several affective scales, ranging from the basic dimensions valence and arousal to the author-based discrete affective dimensions friendliness, spitefulness, and sadness, to aesthetic evaluations of poeticity and liking, as well as the concept of onomatopoeia.

To identify basic textual sources potentially determining these ratings we quantified affective properties of the texts using valence and arousal values from large-scale normative lexical databases at three different basic text levels: We then used these measures as predictor variables in a stepwise multiple regression approach to test how much of the variance in the perceived general affective meaning can be accounted for by these textual variables, and how these influences may vary across different rating dimensions.

Overall, our results from the different regression models show that a prominent portion of the variance in affective and further aesthetic and onomatopoetic ratings of our poems can be accounted for by affective features at the sublexical, lexical, and inter-lexical level. These findings suggest that very basic affective processes play a crucial role in poetry perception. Note that we do not argue that higher-level processes would not matter, they are just not studied in our approach.

The best predictors of the perception of the general affective meaning of the poems—assessed via ratings—were the average lexical valence and arousal values of words—in terms of their deviation from an expected average value—contained in the poems. Pragmatically speaking, this would mean that it is sufficient to put words with specific affective connotations together to create half of the affective impact a poem is able to provoke in the reader.

Again, while this view may appear extremely minimalistic, it is well in line with other findings from reading studies using normal sentences or passages from novels Anderson and McMaster, ; Whissell et al. Beyond the single word level, our study provides a number of novel results for inter-lexical phenomena and how they contribute to the affective reading experience.

From a neuroscientific perspective, Hsu et al. In our data, for instance, the overall ratings of arousal induced by a poem were best predicted not by the average lexical arousal values but rather by specific maxima of lexical arousal. The maximum lexical arousal value in a text is a mathematical constituent of the arousal span max—min and probably the most relevant one, as it represents salient peaks or particularly exciting moments in a text—which well fits the general view on this emotion dimension as an alert system reacting immediately to salient affective input.

Such findings underline the importance of deviation from expected patterns—here the outstanding arousal level of one single word in a text—for foregrounding effects compare with the Neurocognitive Poetics model, NCPM, Jacobs, , a. Furthermore, our novel operationalization of the evolution of affective content throughout a text—correlating lexical affective values with word position—yields a number of interesting results: Instead, poems were perceived as more friendly when words of an increasingly positive character were used toward the final lines of a poem.

We conclude that these correlations between word positions and affective values offer a good proxy for how overall affectivity is being continuously created throughout the course of a poem involving either a classical crescendo or a descent of affective intensity toward the end.

Anton G. Leitner - Wikipedia

In addition, this finding complements well with the established idea that readers naturally exert their greatest reading emphasis at the end of a sentence or passage Gopen and Swan, Last but not least, our data corroborate and extend recent findings on how sublexical phonological features influence affective processes during poetry reading. That is, for instance, valence ratings of poems get more negative, or spitefulness ratings increase, when poems feature particularly many phonological segments of high arousal potential i.

In the present study, using a huge number of predictors from different text levels, we could show that these effects of basic affective tone indeed seem to occur independently of the lexical affective content of the poems, as effects persist even after the very robust effects of lexical affective values have been partialled out in our multiple regression models. Note also that control measures of the basic affective tone —not using the phonologically salient but all phonological segments—only rarely account for significant amounts of variance of the ratings in our multiple regression models and if then only referring to specific subsyllabic units , while the EMOPHON's measures based on phonological salience did so in eight out of nine regression models.

This is strong evidence that phonological salience in combination with phonological iconicity can be considered an important poetic device. Importantly, our data also show that readers are obviously sensitive to phonological salience per se: Subjective ratings of poeticity and onomatopoeia were significantly associated to the number of phonological segments qualified as phonologically salient by the EMOPHON tool.

At the level of rating dimensions as dependent variables—and from a general perspective—our study offers an interesting comparison between rather global evaluations of the general affective meaning of poems using the terms of dimensional emotion models valence and arousal , specific affective dimensions presumably best suitable for the given corpus sadness, spitefulness and friendliness , and the more aesthetic evaluations of liking and poeticity, as well as the further evaluation of onomatopoeia.

Goodness of fit for regression models trying to predict the latter three dimensions was clearly less as compared to the other two groups. This is no surprise, as in the case of valence and arousal ratings, criteria and predictor variables are based on identical operationalizations of affect as all predictors were quantified using valence and arousal values. The author-given labels of spitefulness, sadness, and friendliness deliver even more impressive fits, presumably because they might simply capture the entire variance of affective content of these poems in optimal ways.

Still, our approach offers interesting insights on how more abstract evaluations of poetry such as participants' liking of a poem or the ascription of poeticity and onomatopoeia to a text relate to the basic affective dimensions of valence and arousal at lexical and sublexical textual levels: A remarkable finding is the decrease in general liking ratings of poems with increasing arousal—concerning both the words or concepts dealt with in a poem, and its phonological content also see Aryani et al. As the general arousal level of the poems in the Enzensberger volume is on average very high, a lowered lexical arousal level, as indicated by the regression results for liking, would still be of medium value.

This principle also seems to generalize to the evaluation of poeticity by our participants: Both very high and very low levels of lexical arousal go along with lesser ascriptions of poeticity to the poems. Also at the sublexical level, a rather low arousal level coincides with higher poeticity ratings. A similar pattern is present for the explicit evaluation of phonological content during onomatopoeia ratings: Most interestingly, they also decreased with increasing spreads of lexical valence and arousal.

Again, the focus—at least the conscious one—of our participants on formal features of poetry appeared to be rather disturbed by a too distracting affective variety at the level of semantic content.

Navigation menu

Taken together, while previous studies had reported a range of effects of specific text levels influencing the affective appeal of literature e. What we consider a characteristic strength of the current approach certainly represents a shortcoming when it comes to deliver a comprehensive model of poetry perception: While this alternative approach interestingly matches current computer-based approaches to poetic writing Kirke and Miranda, ; Misztal and Indurkhya, , it does not take into account well established phenomena of, e.

Furthermore, people less experienced with poetry might be less aware of more sophisticated stylistic devices or further meanings on a meta-level. Hence, basic textual features may play a bigger role in forming the general affective meaning of poetry for lay people than for experienced poetry readers.

It would be interesting to investigate through follow-up studies with expert poetry consumers whether the influence of basic textual levels on affective perception would decrease with expertise. Further, also the choice of textual measures could still be extended—for example, integrating morphemic and syntactic text levels—and refined—for example in terms of the inter-lexical measures.

The merit of this study might thus just lie in having made first explorative steps toward investigating—or having opened initial insights on—text-based affective potential functions for several aspects of the general affective meaning.


  • All About Earthquakes Unit Study!
  • Walther von der Vogelweide?
  • Your Step by Step Guide to Confident Public Speaking?
  • www.newyorkethnicfood.com - Lyrische Interpretation by Bianka Schüssler & Sami Abu-Bakr on Apple Books!
  • Anton G. Leitner.

These innovative insights may also compensate methodological disadvantages of our statistical approach using a large number of predictors in stepwise multiple regression. While we opted for this specific method as it seems optimal when screening for the most influential ones among a wide range of possible candidate measures, future studies may apply more fine grained methods to disentangle the details concerning the interplay of a restricted number of variables according to more specific research questions.

Future studies should, in particular, extend our investigations to i the works of other writers—as some of our findings may in theory result from an idiosyncratic writing style of H. Enzensberger, ii non- literary texts or even everyday speech—in different languages, and iii affective ratings from different types of reader groups including expert readers.

Related Articles

In this study we focused on how and to which extent affective connotations of very basic textual measures at the lexical, inter-lexical, and even sublexical level of a poem—that can all be derived from existing normative databases—determine the perception of the general affective meaning of poetry in a way that proves quantifiable beyond the specific context of a given poem, author, or recipient. By applying an exhaustive exploratory regression analysis to a comprehensive corpus of poems and their ratings from hundreds of readers, we found that a significant amount of variance in discrete and dimensional affective ratings of poetry can be accounted for solely by text-based affective measures from different levels of processing.

In all of the presented statistical models—focusing on different aspects of the general affective meaning —variance of each rating dimension is significantly accounted for by affective properties of several text levels: Thus, our research brings together previous accounts on specific effects of single text levels, showing how they may co-exist each in their own right or interact to constitute the complex holistic framework of poetry perception.

SU collected and analyzed the data, as well as wrote the main part of the manuscript. AA was responsible for some of the computational aspects, especially regarding the Emophon tool developed by him, and offered important critical feedback. MK initiated the idea to use the poem corpus and gave helpful input from her philological perspective throughout the whole research process. AJ gave important input regarding the theoretical framework of the study. All authors substantially contributed to the conception and interpretation of the work, revised it critically, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

We conducted a non-experimental, voluntary online survey, in which people had to read and judge poems. In the instructions we told the participants that they can skip the survey any time they want to. If they had any questions regarding the survey they could contact us any time e-mail addresses provided.

There was only one participant of the online survey who was only 17 years old. We did not have any additional instructions for minors or their parents. But we assume that rating poetry does not pose a significant difference between teenagers and adults. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: Europe PMC requires Javascript to function effectively. Enzensberger had labeled as either "friendly," "sad," or "spiteful. The snippet could not be located in the article text. This may be because the snippet appears in a figure legend, contains special characters or spans different sections of the article.

Published online Jan Jacobs , 1, 2, 4 and Markus Conrad 1, 5. This article was submitted to Language Sciences, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology.

Read Das Buch Der Liebe: Lyrische Und Dramatische Dichtungen 2015

Received Feb 27; Accepted Dec The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author s or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. This article has been cited by other articles in PMC.

Abstract The literary genre of poetry is inherently related to the expression and elicitation of emotion via both content and form. Introduction Emotional impact constitutes an important aspect of poetry Turner and Poeppel, ; Cupchik, ; van Peer et al. Lexical effects on general affective meaning Lexical affective meaning has been shown to be of reliable predictive potential for the affective perception of different types of texts Anderson and McMaster, ; Whissell et al. Sublexical effects on general affective meaning Poetry inherently involves the structuring of sound, which is why it is important to consider the phonological composition at the sublexical level—also and especially when investigating the emotional impact of poetry.

Participants German native speakers were recruited through a post on the institute's website and a diversity of Facebook webpages. Procedure and variables General affective meaning ratings were acquired via an online survey using the QuestBack Unipark software. A minimum of 15 complete ratings for each poem were acquired on each of the following eight dimensions—presented to participants in randomized order: Ratings of Valence and Arousal—Linking our Approach to Psychological Emotion Models — Valence 1 on a 7-point scale ranging from—3 very negative via 0 neutral to 3 positive.

Multiple regression The rating variables including the absolute value of valence were used as dependent variables in a multiple regression approach. Lexical predictors All poem texts were PoS part-of-speech tagged to identify the word forms and infinitives of each word. As example, the formula for the sigma factor of lexical arousal looks as follows: Inter-lexical predictors The inter-lexical variables that we included in the analysis are thought to reflect tensions and dynamics within a text.

Standard deviations and spans of all words' valence and arousal values may serve as a proxy for the general affective spread of a poem: For the number of salient phonological segments that exceed their confidence intervals, we used the — N s of salient onsets, N s of salient nuclei, N s of salient codas as well as the N s of all salient subsyllabic segments altogether in each case weighted by the length of the respective poem. Table 1 Means M and standard deviations SD of the rating variables for each author-given affective category N being the number of poems rated. Open in a separate window.

Table 2 Bivariate correlation coefficients between all rating variables. Table 3 Predictors of the basic affective dimensions' ratings. The bold number indicates the respective overall cumulative R 2 corrected for each regression model. Table 4 Predictors of the discrete affective concepts' ratings.

Table 5 Predictors of the two aesthetic and the onomatopoeia ratings. SD, standard deviation, … , absolute value; Indicators of significance: Discussion This study investigates to which extent affective connotations at the rather basic textual dimensions of phonological units and single words or the relative positions of the latter influence the overall affective perception of poetry. Limitations and future prospects What we consider a characteristic strength of the current approach certainly represents a shortcoming when it comes to deliver a comprehensive model of poetry perception: Conclusion In this study we focused on how and to which extent affective connotations of very basic textual measures at the lexical, inter-lexical, and even sublexical level of a poem—that can all be derived from existing normative databases—determine the perception of the general affective meaning of poetry in a way that proves quantifiable beyond the specific context of a given poem, author, or recipient.

Conflict of interest statement The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. Supplementary material The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: Computer assisted modeling of affective tone in written documents. Extracting salient sublexical units from written texts: Measuring basic affective tone in poems via phonological saliency and iconicity.

Arts 10 , — P is for Happiness, N is for Sadness: Can emotional valence in stories be determined from words? Johns Hopkins University Press. When we like what we know - A parametric fMRI analysis of beauty and familiarity. Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. The word frequency effect: The time course of emotion effects in first and second language processing: Poetics 23 , — The Perm State Institute of Culture; , 65— Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech.

Vorschule Der Asthetik, Vol. Word 17 , — Aesthetic emotions and reality.